There is perhaps no field of scientific study in which more progress has been made
Recent field research has shown, however, that the elusive data which these theorists have so diligently sought has in fact been lying under our very feet all along. In this article, we uncover
-emic |
Which of us, in our preteen years, has not set out on a quest for arrowheads beside a countryside stream, and found him or herself rewarded with a stone that, while not quite an arrowhead, had clearly been shaped by human labor? We can now reveal that the stones we all found are not mere evidence of tool use, but are the actual record of our pre-
Archeologists have always assumed that primitive paleolithic proto-
But this is demonstrably incorrect. The early chipped stones were not primitive tools, but were in fact primitive words. Early stone chipping behavior is not an example of tool-
Consider, for example, the following renderings of stone words which have been in the first author’s private collection since childhood:
‘mother’ |
‘aunt’ |
The obvious structural similarity between these two magnificent specimens is due to the semantic relationship of their referents, not (as was previously assumed) to some shared function in their form as physical tools. A stone age conversation involving the hefting of such intricate and painstakingly-
Fast forward a hundred thousand years or more: Mesopotamia
However, the expense of bronze kept it out of the hands of the common people. Those unable to afford bronze were forced to continue to use stone. This, we contend, is likely the original high-
With the advent of the Iron Age, the crude tools and crude words of the Bronze Age were abandoned in favor of the more readily worked iron. But iron words soon proved to be too heavy for practical use. One of the many revelations of our theory is that the general adoption of iron lead directly to human speech.
The consequences of this brilliant hypothesis range from the intriguingly trivial to the breathtakingly iconoclastic. Interestingly, phrases such as “hurling hard words” and “weighty words” are linguistic shadows of the past, probably coming down to us from the Stone Age and Iron Age, respectively. More dramatically, linguistic change itself changed abruptly about 3500 years ago when iron came to the fore and then, shortly thereafter, oral speech became the norm
Moreover, language change before this shift must have proceeded at a much slower rate, as even the
carelessly tossed out words of an ancestor, in heaps on the ground, might have spoken clearly to a stone-
-etic
early humans must have thought twice about abandoning perfectly good existing vocabulary, when the alternative was hours of work chipping or casting a suitable neologism.
Shades of Grey
by Piotr Pablo Paulsen
Borrowing words from another human population would have been easy
Most conversation itself must have been more leisurely, thoughtful, and less hurried, when literally crafting a well considered reply could take hours or days. Of course, our modern penchant for reaching for easy clichés and well worn turns of phrase almost certainly hearkens back to our ancestors’ likely practice of keeping a pile of pre-
As we have already discussed, our inability to reconstruct proto-
We expect our revelation to usher in several new fields and subfields. Geo-
Given time, these brave new fields of study will specialize further, as Geology and Linguistics departments may one day come to offer joint programs specializing in the interpretation of particular geo-
All told, this solid-
The Quotta and the Quottiod: Punctuation Designed for Linguists, by Linguists |
|
Evidential Complexity and Language Loss in Pinnacle Sherpa |
|
SpecGram Vol CLI, No 4 Contents |