Divine Unification Grammar--Ǣlfgār λ. Garcia Linguist of Fortune -- JLSSCNC Vol I, No 1 Contents Exploring Penguin Causatives--Robert F. Scott

Discourse Antipassivity in Indonesian

Consider the following examples from Mandarin (Chen 1990):
(1)    Wǒ búhwèi shwō Jūnggwó hwà.
‘I don’t know how to speak Chinese.’
(2) Nǐman kàn shū.
‘You (pl.) read books.’
(3) Tāde péngyǒu mài bǐ, búmài jwōdz.
‘His friend sells pencils but does not sell tables.’

Likewise the following examples from Classical Latin (Cicero -63):

(4)    O tempora, o mores!
‘Oh the times, oh the morals!’
(5)    Mosne maiorum?
‘The customs of our ancestors?’
And also the data below from Alabama (Davis 1989):
(6)    salin roykan ittaasat fayahlo
‘Sally and Roy stopped.’
(7)    alikchon stiliichabannakha
‘I wanted to become a doctor.’
Taken together, all of these examples make obvious the proper analysis for Indonesian sentences of the sort exemplified by number (8), below (from Slater 1989):
(8)    lakilaki
‘the boys’
We might note in passing the existence of languages which use another structural device to encode the same sort of meaning as the structure found in the sentences above; e.g., Martian (Hockett 1955):
(9)    KAHtah
‘setting of Deimos’
And, of course, English, which exactly follows the Martian pattern:
(10)    Yesterday I went to the beach, but the water was a bit too cold for swimming.
As far as I know, these two distinct structures for the encoding of this semantics are the only ones attested, making the choice of which structure to use an important typological parameter.

Mokele MbembeKinshasa Abnormal University

Divine Unification Grammar--Ǣlfgār λ. Garcia
Exploring Penguin Causatives--Robert F. Scott
Linguist of Fortune -- JLSSCNC Vol I, No 1 Contents